advertisement

Pages

Monday, June 29, 2009

Konrad Lorenz, Imprinting & Functional Brain Imaging

The other day, I discovered footage of goslings swimming behind the great Austrian ethologist Konrad Lorenz rowing a boat. Konrad Lorenz completed doctorates in medicine and biology. This combination was common for anatomists of his time, because many pursued research in comparative anatomy of the animal kingdom in addition to teaching gross anatomy. The Professor of Anatomy at my alma mater, Professor Dr. Dr. Dietrich Starck, displayed an elephant heart preserved in a huge glass jar in his office and was known around the world for editing the Handbooks of Zoology. Peculiarly, Konrad Lorenz did not follow the traditional career of an anatomist, but was drawn to behavioral studies. I remember his astounding manipulations of animal behavior from my secondary school days. We were shown movies of his work that IWF distributed to schools on big reels of regular eight celluloid film. The movies were silent. Subtext was added. Sound would not have been of much help. The projector rattled incredibly loud. Its bulb heated the classroom to a sweat. Despite, we loved watching those movies.

In one of them, Lorenz showed us how goslings became fixated in their behavior onto the object they found near them first after they hatched, following it wherever it went. The behavior was known from other nidifugous birds like chickens. In nature, the first object is commonly the mother. Lorenz substituted the mother with himself, and the ducklings followed him instead. Elaborate features proved unnecessary. The goslings would also accept a moving ball or a stroller. Most importantly, the object had to be present within 13-16 hours after hatching. Beyond this critical period the goslings would not attach to the object. The process is known as imprinting. Imprinted geese would maintain the behavioral object fixation for many months. Lorenz concluded that although the stimulus releasing the behavior could vary, the stereotypical behavior itself was enacted instinctively and must be innate, because the hatchlings did not have the opportunity to learn the behavior from any one else. You may wish to see Lorenz in action here:



Lorenz gained his fundamental insights with minimally invasive observation. The observer deliberately hid from the participants in the research and, thus, interfered with the observations in the least possible fashion. In reciprocation to Lorenz applying his observations on animal behavior to humanity, minimally invasive methods of observation found acceptance in behavioral psychology. My son attended The Susan Gray School at Peabody College in Nashville, Tennessee. Very much in harmony with the title of Lorenz's famed book "Behind The Mirror: A Search for a Natural History of Human Knowledge", observation rooms are wedged between the children's classrooms, permitting scientists to closely observe through one-way mirrors the interactions of the unwitting children with toys, peers and care givers. Susan Gray and Nicholas Hobbs were highly successful in using this method to search for early indicators of abnormal behavioral development, opening new avenues for early childhood interventions. Their methods are still in use today.

In the early 1980s, Henning Scheich and others provided evidence that the connections between nerve cells in the brain are particularly modified in imprinted birds as another example of brain plasticity. Guinea fowl were imprinted onto objects emitting tones of particular pitch. Using the autoradiographic deoxyglucose method of Sokoloff and others (1987) to image local metabolic brain activity, the authors showed pronounced nerve cell activation in the birds auditory forebrain related to the pitch of imprint. The findings suggest that lasting nerve cell connections develop during the critical period of imprinting, embedding a memory trace of the release stimulus for the behavior. The findings were published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (Maier and Scheich, 1983). Subsequent work established that the density of protrusions on nerve cell processes, known as dendritic spines, diminished on a particular type of cell in one of the activated brain regions (Wallhäusser and Scheich, 1987). Excitatory endings from other nerve cells mainly terminate on dendritic spines. Therefore, selective pruning of excitatory nerve endings on specific nerve cells during the critical period may underlie imprinting-related brain activation.

Douglas Spalding described filial imprinting already in the 19th century, perhaps providing the earliest notion of a behavior dependent on a critical period during postnatal development. In his work, Konrad Lorenz elaborated on this notion, providing a foundation for Torsten Wiesel's and David Hubel's discovery of a critical period for plasticity of cerebral cortex. They discovered interlocked domains in visual cerebral cortex, in which nerve cells responded predominantly to the stimulation of either one eye. Because the dominance of monocular input spanned the cortical thickness, the domains are known as ocular dominance columns. Occlusion of one eye during the critical period resulted in the expansion of the columns receiving input from the unimpeded eye. The effect was reversible as long as the eye occlusion was switched during the critical period (Wiesel and Hubel, 1965). These findings laid the groundwork for the exploration of the nerve cell mechanisms involved in brain plasticity and behavior.

Lorenz shared the Nobel Prize with Niko Tinbergen and Karl von Frisch in 1973 and Wiesel and Hubel with Roger Sperry in 1981. I have written about Sperry's discoveries on split brains in my post dated Apr. 28, 2009.

Addendum
  • Konrad Lorenz's student Irenäus Eibl-Eibesfeldt used a camera lens with a mirror that permitted him to film the facial expressions of unsuspecting bystanders unnoticed. Taking pictures in this fashion, he and his colleagues were able to identify archetypal facial expressions commonly used across cultures, suggesting that also humans share innate behaviors. Eibl-Eibesfeldt authored an authoritative book on his observations entitled "Human Ethology". The raising of the eye brows to signal readiness for social interaction represents one example shown on the home page of the Film Archive of Human Ethology. (10/28/09).
References
Related Posts


Monday, June 22, 2009

In Remembrance of Phil Browning

Today I wish to commemorate the fifth anniversary of the passing of a great friend and teacher. Philip Juan Browning was raised by a hard working family in Gary, Indiana. His mother saw to it with iron will that her children finished high school and proceeded to college. Phil attended Fisk University in Nashville, Tennessee, exactly at a time when Black Power gained traction. Philip became an activist in the Black Panther youth movement.

After some less encouraging experiences and contemplation, Phil decided to leave the Panthers and take the path Dr. King had pointed out. He opted to run the gamut of institutions, becoming one of at the time select number of African Americans accepted for medical training at Tufts University School of Medicine. He went on to Harvard Medical School for training in internal medicine at Brigham and Women's Hospital. He received his training in oncology and hematology at the Dana Faber Cancer Institute in Boston and the National Cancer Institute (NCI) outside Washington, D.C. At the NCI, he worked in the laboratory of Robert Gallo, under whose leadership the first test for AIDS was developed. Phil eventually accepted a faculty position at Vanderbilt University to continue his research and practice medicine.

Phil was not only a talented scientist and a great physician. He also was a man with a mission. He embarked on his career with fierce determination. He wanted to prove to the world that it was indeed possible in this country with tenacity and perseverance to take the most difficult hurdles and achieve the extraordinary, only to put his achievements into the service of people who needed his help. Phil cared about his students. He was an active member of his church where he mentored the young to the very end of his day.

I remember enlightened and sometimes heated conversations with Phil over lunch about science, Chicago, the Panthers, the African American experience, and contemporary politics. Based on my teutonic upbringing, I am predisposed to mulling decisions many times over. I tend to look for reasons why not to do something, posing the question, "is this a wise thing to do?" Phil would look out of the window with a frown until I was finished with my litany of excuses, turn back to me, flashing a huge smile, and reply: "Why not?"

Phil passed peacefully after a long and valiant battle with colon cancer. He never lost faith. In my deepest moments of doubt, I still hear Phil exclaim: "Why not?"

A memorial fund was instituted in his honor at Vanderbilt University to support African American post graduate students and a clinical service will be named for him this year.

I wrote a poem for him on the occasion of his 50th birthday, a bright and sunny day at the beginning of May that a big crowd of friends and family enjoyed in his backyard. Phil sang Motown songs for his wife Renee and was really good at it.

Be patient and let the magic ink do its work: